Speaker: Danny Yihan Jia @pitque
Affiliation: Boston University
Co-authors: James J. Cummings
Title: Effect of Narrative Outcomes on Viewer Engagement After Exposure: A Longitudinal Study
Abstract: The use of multiple narrative endings has become common in the creation of fiction, film, and television, yet little is known about how they affect audiencesâ experiences. This proposed longitudinal study examines how different film endingsâprotagonist success or antagonist successâinfluence viewersâ retrospective imaginative involvement, a reflective engagement process extending beyond initial narrative exposure. Specifically, it explores how differing resolutions shape viewersâ perceptions of the film (e.g., enjoyment and appreciation), and whether these perceptions contribute to RII over one month. The findings could offer insights into the psychological mechanisms behind narrative engagement and provide practical implications for creators to craft narratives that enhance engagement.
Long abstract
Audiencesâ engagement with narratives is often conceptualized as confined to the temporal bounds of the narrative encounter (Slater et al., 2018). However, perspectives on retrospective imaginative involvement (RII) suggest audiences may reflect on narrative elements post-exposure (Slater et al., 2018; Ewoldsen et al., 2021; Sethi et al., 2022). This post-exposure contemplation indicates a narrativeâs impact extends beyond immediate consumption, prompting psychological involvement long after the experience (Sethi et al., 2022).
Both anecdotal evidence and empirical research support the prevalence of RII (Ewoldsen et al., 2021; Sherrick et al., 2022; Ulusoy et al., 2022). However, past studies have predominantly relied on survey methodologies and correlational designs, limiting the ability to make causal claims about how in-the-moment experiences influence post-exposure engagement. Ulusoy et al. (2022) called for experimental designs to address these gaps. This study responds by investigating how different story resolutions impact RII.
Story Resolution as Potential Antecedent to RII
Writers use various ending techniques to engage audiences (Barber, 2010; Neupert, 1995; Smorti, 2004), and these endings shape audiencesâ perception and memory. Classic âhappy endingsâ offer closure and satisfaction (e.g., The Shawshank Redemption). Conversely, ending with the antagonistâs victory can defy expectations, evoking unease or frustration (Zillmann, 2014). These choices create lasting psychological effects beyond the final scene.
When a narrative provides a definitive resolution, the toneâwhether favorable to the protagonist or antagonistâmay shape future imaginative involvement. Protagonist victories generate feelings of pleasure, satisfaction, or relief (Krakowiak, 2024; Krakowiak & Tsay-Vogel, 2015). This may lead to static-RII, where audiences reflect on the narrativeâs facts without feeling the need to ârewriteâ the outcome (Ewoldsen et al., 2022; Ulusoy et al., 2022).
Conversely, antagonist victories may reduce enjoyment and create dissatisfaction, prompting dynamic-RII. This involves reimagining or interpreting alternative outcomes where the protagonist succeeds (Schibler et al., 2024; Ulusoy et al., 2022). Dissatisfaction drives viewers to revisit themes, characters, and events to explore unfulfilled possibilities and seek cognitive resolution.
H1: A story ending with the protagonistâs victory (vs. the antagonistâs victory) will lead to higher static-RII, mediated by increased satisfaction with the narrative resolution.
H2: A story ending with the antagonistâs victory (vs. the protagonistâs victory) will lead to higher dynamic-RII, mediated by lower satisfaction with the narrative resolution.
Linking Enjoyment and Appreciation to RII.
Narrative engagement often encompasses two primary evaluative dimensions: enjoyment and appreciation (Lewis et al., 2014; Oliver & Bartsch, 2010). Enjoyment is typically associated with hedonic pleasure, arising from positive emotions such as satisfaction, relief, and amusement during the narrative experience (Krakowiak & Tsay-Vogel, 2015). In contrast, appreciation is often associated with mixed affects, poignancy, contemplation, and/or challenging cognitions (Oliver & Bartsch, 2010; Oliver & Raney, 2014; Oliver, 2008).
Both enjoyment and appreciation can be influenced by narrative resolution. For example, Krakowiak (2024) found positive character outcomes tend to increase enjoyment and appreciation. Additionally, heightened enjoyment of a narrative outcome was linked to decreased coping behaviors, whereas heightened appreciation was associated with increased coping behaviors (Krakowiak, 2024). RII is particularly relevant in this context, as it can serve as a coping mechanism to fulfill unmet psychological needs (Sherrick et al., 2022; Ulusoy et al., 2022). Audiences who experience dissatisfaction with a narrative endingâsuch as one where the antagonist prevailsâmay be more motivated to engage in RII to alleviate frustration or unresolved tension. By reimagining alternative narrative outcomes, viewers may find ways to mentally resolve the dissatisfaction induced by an unfavorable ending.
Furthermore, narrative viewers often reflect on enjoyment and appreciation after the narrative experience, and these retrospective evaluations change over time (Ott & Slater, 2024). As a result, these evaluations can influence RII at different stages post-exposure. While enjoyment may drive static-RII after a satisfying ending, appreciationâs role in static and dynamic-RII is less clear.
H3: A story ending with the protagonistâs victory (vs. the antagonistâs victory) will lead to higher levels of enjoyment.
RQ1: Will different story resolutions lead to different levels of appreciation?
RQ2: How will enjoyment and appreciation of the film contribute to RII over the course of one month after narrative exposure?
Methods
Stimuli
Participants will watch the 2017 film Get Out. The narrative follows a Black man who uncovers a sinister plot during a visit to his white girlfriendâs family estate. The film explores themes of racial exploitation and systemic injustice. Two endings will be used: a) the theatrical ending where the protagonist escapes, and b) an ending where he survives but is imprisoned for self-defense (https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=A3JS7_OcPWQ). Participants will be randomly assigned to one of the two conditions.
Procedure
After viewing, participants will complete a questionnaire measuring their 1) satisfaction with the ending, 2) enjoyment and appreciation, and 3) personality traits that might influence RII (e.g., trait curiosity). A one-month longitudinal design will be employed to better capture RII and retrospective enjoyment and appreciation. Separate questionnaires measuring RII, enjoyment, and appreciation will be administered three days, one week, two weeks, and one month after viewing.
The study will be conducted in January 2025, with data collection concluding by February 2025. We are submitting this as a paper because data will be collected before the IGEL conference.
References
Barber, S. (2010). Abandoned images: film and filmâs end. Reaktion Books.
Berg, C. R. (2006). A taxonomy of alternative plots in recent films: Classifying the âTarantino effectâ. Film Criticism, 31(1/2), 5-61.
Ewoldsen, D. R., Busselle, R., Sethi, N., & Slater, M. D. (04 2021). 734735Retrospective Imaginative Involvement and Entertainment Narratives: Initial Forays. In The Oxford Handbook of Entertainment Theory. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190072216.013.38
Ewoldsen, D. R., Hoewe, J., & Grady, S. (2022). A cognitive processing framework for media interpretation. Journal of Media Psychology.
Glaser, M., Choi, Y. K., & Baumgartner, H. (2024). (Un) Bothered by the Storyâs Ambiguity: How Individual Differences in Consumersâ Need for Closure Affect Transportation and Brand Attitude in Narrative Ads. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 9(1), 46-57.
Koch, G. (1997). â Against All Oddsâ or the Will to Survive: Moral Conclusions from Narrative Closure. History and Memory, 9(1/2), 393-408.
Krakowiak, K. M. (2024). How It Ends: Exploring How Narrative Closure and Character Outcomes in Series Finales Relate to Viewer Responses and Coping Behaviors. Mass Communication and Society, 1-28.
Krakowiak, K. M., & Tsay-Vogel, M. (2015). The dual role of morally ambiguous characters: Examining the effect of morality salience on narrative responses. Human Communication Research, 41(3), 390-411.
Lewis, R. J., Tamborini, R., & Weber, R. (2014). Testing a dual-process model of media enjoyment and appreciation. Journal of Communication, 64(3), 397-416.
Lu, A. S., Green, M. C., Sousa, C. V., Hwang, J., Lee, I. M., Thompson, D., & Baranowski, T. (2023). To Pause With a Cliffhanger or a Temporary Closure? The Differential Impact of Serial Versus Episodic Narratives on Childrenâs Physical Activity Behaviors. Communication Research, 00936502231166091.
Neupert, R. J. (1995). The end: narration and closure in the cinema. Wayne State University Press.
Oliver, M. B. (2008). Tender affective states as predictors of entertainment preference. Journal of Communication, 58(1), 40-61.
Oliver, M. B., & Bartsch, A. (2010). Appreciation as audience response: Exploring entertainment gratifications beyond hedonism. Human communication research, 36(1), 53-81.
Oliver, M. B., & Raney, A. A. (2011). Entertainment as pleasurable and meaningful: Identifying hedonic and eudaimonic motivations for entertainment consumption. Journal of Communication, 61(5), 984-1004.
Ott, J. M., & Slater, M. D. (2024). Postexposure engagement with more and less eudaimonic films: 10-year patterns of response and the role of parasocial relationship and retrospective imaginative involvement. Psychology of Popular Media, 13(1), 150.
Schibler, K., Hahn, L., & Green, M. C. (2024). Investigating Responses to Narrative Cliffhangers Using Affective Disposition Theory. Media Psychology, 27(1), 1-25.
Sethi, N., Grady, S. M., Ulusoy, E., Baldwin, J., & Ewoldsen, D. R. (2022). What do we do with narratives after the fact? Exploring dimensions of Retrospective Imaginative Involvement. Communication Reports, 35(2), 106-119.
Sherrick, B., Hoewe, J., & Ewoldsen, D. R. (2022). Using narrative media to satisfy intrinsic needs: Connecting parasocial relationships, retrospective imaginative involvement, and self-determination theory. Psychology of Popular Media, 11(3), 266.
Slater, M. D., Ewoldsen, D. R., & Woods, K. W. (2018). Extending conceptualization and measurement of narrative engagement after-the-fact: Parasocial relationship and retrospective imaginative involvement. Media Psychology, 21(3), 329-351.
Smorti, A. (2004). Narrative strategies for interpreting stories with incongruent endings. Narrative Inquiry, 14(1), 141-167.
Thissen, B. A., Menninghaus, W., & Schlotz, W. (2021). The pleasures of reading fiction explained by flow, presence, identification, suspense, and cognitive involvement. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 15(4), 710.
Ulusoy, E., Sethi, N., Baldwin, J., Grady, S. M., & Ewoldsen, D. R. (2022). Canât stop thinking about Star Wars and The Office: antecedents of retrospective imaginative involvement. Human Communication Research, 48(4), 622-633.
Zillmann, D. (2014). Anatomy of suspense. In The entertainment functions of television (pp. 133-163). Psychology Press.